Processing Capacity Limits Are Not Explained by Storage Limits

نویسندگان

  • Graeme S. Halford
  • Steven Phillips
  • William H. Wilson
چکیده

Cowan’s review shows that a short-term memory limit of four items is consistent with a wide range of phenomena in the field. However he does not explain that limit, whereas an existing theory does offer an explanation for capacity limitations. Furthermore processing capacity limits cannot be reduced to storage limits as Cowan claims. In his excellent review, Cowan concludes that short term memory storage is limited to four items, and he notes this corresponds to the limit in processing capacity defined by Halford, Wilson & Phillips (BBS, 1998). Furthermore his conclusion that the limit is in the number of integrated objects, independent of the complexity of each, agrees well with the observation of HW&P (1998) that humans are limited to relating four entities, irrespective of their complexity. However these correspondences do not imply that processing limits can be subsumed under storage limits, as Cowan claims. The fact that the size of the limit is 4 in both cases is not a strong argument for identification because, given that the limit is small, the same number could occur in both contexts by coincidence. Alternatively, storage and processing systems could be distinct but with equal capacities to facilitate transfer from one to the other. There are a number of reasons why processing cannot be subsumed under storage. To take a straightforward example, there clearly is a difference between simply holding the numbers 7 and 4 in short term store, and adding them to yield the sum, 11. In general storage, in the sense of internal representation, is a prerequisite for processing, but cognitive processing cannot be reduced to storage. Furthermore higher cognitive processes require representations that have properties beyond those required for storage, including omnidirectional access and analogical mapping (HW&P, 1998). Cowan’s position is that a concurrent short term memory load can be held in the activated portion of long term memory while other information is being processed in the focus of attention. Lack of interference between processing and short term storage is explained because the focus of attention can be devoted to either storage or processing, but need not be devoted to both at once. However this still implies that storage and processing are distinct, and also implies there would be no tradeoff between the two. It is not fundamentally different from the position of HW&P (1998). Cowan offers no explanation for the limit he observes in storage capacity, whereas HW&P (1998) offer a natural explanation for processing capacity limits. In this model, conceptual complexity is defined by the arity, or number of arguments that can be bound into a single relation. Human adults are typically limited to processing one quaternary relation in parallel. Each component of the relation is represented by a vector, and the binding is represented by the tensor product of the vectors. Thus the binary relational instance larger(elephant,mouse) is represented by vlarger⊗velephant⊗vmouse. The rank of the tensor product is one more than the arity of the relation. The more complex relations are represented by tensor products of higher rank, the greater complexity of which explains why more complex relations are associated with higher processing load. However the size of the component vectors has much less effect on processing load, so the fact that the limit is not related to the size of the entities is also explained. Thus, in terms of our relational model, there is a limit on tensor rank entailed by the rapid growth of the number of tensor units as rank increases. Given that a short term memory store of capacity 4 is connected to a tensor-like system for processing, the limit of 4 on store size is a consequence of the fact that for most cognitive tasks, processing of the objects in the store is a necessity. The links between storage and processing phenomena are worth exploring. In Section 2, Cowan argues that the unity of conscious awareness implies the contents of attended channels should be integrated or combined. Similarly, category clusters (discussed in 2.7 and 3.4.2) imply a link between instances of the category. Cowan further contends, in 3.1.3, that the short term storage limit is observed only with items recalled in correct serial positions. Given that the slots of a relation are identified, serial position can be coded as a relation ordered-items(item1,item2,item3,item4). The observation of no limit with free recall would then suggest that it is ability to represent the relation, rather than the items, that is subject to the limit. This would appear to be consistent with the relational complexity theory of HW&P (1998). Furthermore, it clearly points to explaining storage limits in terms of complexity of relations that can be represented. This would also explain the finding of Nairne (1991, referred to by Cowan in 3.4.3) that errors occur up to three positions from the correct position. The reason would be that the items are represented as a quaternary relation, which contains only four slots. The further finding, in 3.4.5 that participants could predict the 7 item from items 3, 4, 6 may also indicate that the task is represented as a quaternary relation. These phenomena indicate links between entities that are important, but the nature of these links is not really clear, and the issue is clouded by the lack of a well specified theory in Cowan’s paper. Some properties of relational knowledge defined by HW&P (1998) seem to be involved in the phenomena discussed above, but it is not clear that they all are. We could define the relational instances fruit(apple, banana, orange, pear) and fruit(lychee,pineapple,passionfruit,guava), etc. Organizing memory storage as quaternary relations in this way would account for recall of items in clusters of four. However it would also predict a lot of other properties of relational knowledge that Cowan has not demonstrated. For example, relational knowledge has the property of omni-directional access (HW&P, 1998) which means that, given any n-1 components of a relational instance, the remaining component can be retrieved. Thus, given the quaternary relation proportion(4,2,?,3) we can determine that the missing component must be “6” because it is necessary to complete the proportion 4/2 = 6/3. However it is far from clear that category clusters share this property. If given a list [apple, banana, ? , pear] there is no particular reason why we should recall “orange”. Thus category clusters do not entail the kind of constraints that are entailed in relations. Another property of relational knowledge is that analogical mappings can be formed between corresponding relational instances (Holyoak & Thagard, 1995). Again, it is not clear that analogies can be formed between category clusters. Storage is not a simple, unitary matter, but can take many forms. Furthermore, the form in which information is stored affects the form in which it is processed. Some of the possibilities, together with possible implementation in neural nets, are: Item storage – implemented as a vector vi of activation values over a set of neural units. Associative links between items, implemented as connection weights between units in different vectors. Superposition of items – implemented as summation of item vectors. This is tantamount to a prototype. Superimposed items bound to a category label, such as fruit(apple) + fruit(banana) + fruit(orange) + fruit(pear). This is equivalent to a unary relation and can be represented by a Rank 2 tensor vfruit⊗vapple + vfruit⊗vorange + vfruit⊗vorange + vfruit⊗vpear Item-position bindings: ordered-fruit(first,apple) + ordered-fruit (second,orange) + , . . , + orderedfruit(fourth,pear)}. This is a binary relational instance and can be implemented by the tensor product vordered-fruit⊗vfirst⊗vapple + vordered-fruit⊗vsecond⊗vorange + . . . + vordered-fruit⊗vfourth⊗vpear Binding items into n-ary relations where n has a maximum value 4. This can be implemented by a tensor up to Rank 5: vfruit⊗vapple⊗vorange⊗....⊗vpear These representations have different characteristics. They permit different retrieval operations, and impose different processing loads. Importantly, at least some of their properties can be captured by neural net models. The Rank n tensor would explain why processing load increases with the number of entities related, and consequently suggests why the capacity limit tends to be low. However the earlier representations are not sensitive to processing load in this way. It should be clear from these examples that storage and process are intimately related, and that a theory of capacity must include both aspects of computation. However, while their interaction may be complex, it is not arbitrary. Our theory specifies a unique set of properties for processes involving relations of different arities.

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity.

Miller (1956) summarized evidence that people can remember about seven chunks in short-term memory (STM) tasks. However, that number was meant more as a rough estimate and a rhetorical device than as a real capacity limit. Others have since suggested that there is a more precise capacity limit, but that it is only three to five chunks. The present target article brings together a wide variety o...

متن کامل

A New Algorithm for Determining Ultimate Pit Limits Based on Network Optimization

One of the main concerns of the mining industry is to determine ultimate pit limits. Final pit is a collection of blocks, which can be removed with maximum profit while following restrictions on the slope of the mine’s walls. The size, location and final shape of an open-pit are very important in designing the location of waste dumps, stockpiles, processing plants, access roads and other surfac...

متن کامل

Increasing the hydrogen storage capacity of single-walled carbon nanotube (SWNT) through facile impregnation by TiO2, ZrO2 and ZnO nanocatalysts

Various nanocomposites of TiO2, ZnO and ZrO2 decorated single wall Carbon nanotubes (SWNTs) were fabricated by facile and template free continuous ultrasonication/stirring of virgin metal oxide nanopowders and SWNTs in ethanol under UV-light illumination. The TEM micrographs showed that nanoparticles (NPs) were uniformly dispersed and bonded on the surface of SWNTs. The results of XRD as well a...

متن کامل

Misaligned and Polarity-Reversed Faces Determine Face-specific Capacity Limits

Previous research using flanker paradigms suggests that peripheral distracter faces are automatically processed when participants have to classify a single central familiar target face. These distracter interference effects disappear when the central task contains additional anonymous (non-target) faces that load the search for the face target, but not when the central task contains additional ...

متن کامل

Temporal buffering and visual capacity: The time course of object formation underlies capacity limits in visual cognition

Capacity limits are a hallmark of visual cognition. The upper boundary of our ability to individuate and remember objects is well known but-despite its central role in visual information processing-not well understood. Here, we investigated the role of temporal limits in the perceptual processes of forming "object files." Specifically, we examined the two fundamental mechanisms of object file f...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2001